Citation: | ZHANG Jijia, YE Can, WANG Ting. Ecological Environment and Humanistic Environment Shaping Individual Cultural Orientation: Evidence from Middle School Students in Dong and Oroqen Areas[J]. Journal of South China normal University (Social Science Edition), 2022, (4): 59-71. |
[1] |
HOFSTEDE G H. Culture's consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations[M]. London: Sage Publications, Incorporated, 2001: 1-29.
|
[2] |
TARAS V, SARALA R, MUCHINSKY P, et al. Opposite ends of the same stick? Multi-method test of the dimensionality of individualism and collectivism[J]. Journal of cross-cultural psychology, 2014, 45(2): 213-245. doi: 10.1177/0022022113509132
|
[3] |
TRIANDIS H C. The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts[J]. Psychological review, 1989, 96(3): 506-520. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506
|
[4] |
TWENGE J M, ABEBE E M, CAMPBELL W K. Fitting in or standing out: trends in American parents' choices for children's names, 1880—2007[J]. Social psychological and personality science, 2010, 1(1): 19-25. doi: 10.1177/1948550609349515
|
[5] |
霍夫斯坦德. 文化之重: 价值、行为、体制和组织的跨国比较: 第2版[M]. 上海: 上海外语教育出版社, 1980: 56-64.
|
[6] |
OYSERMAN D, COON H M, KEMMELMEIER M. Rethinking individualism and collectivism: evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses[J]. Psychological bulletin, 2002, 128(1): 3-72. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.1.3
|
[7] |
INGLEHART R, BAKER W E. Modernization, cultural change and the persistence of traditional values[J]. American sociological review, 2001, 65(1): 19-51.
|
[8] |
DUFFY S, UCHIDA Y, KITAYAMA S. Symbolic self-inflation: a cross-cultural comparison[M]. Camden, NJ: Rutgers University-Camden, 2008: 101-103.
|
[9] |
NISBETT R E, PENG K, CHOI I, et al. Culture and systems of thought: holistic versus analytic cognition[J]. Psychological review, 2001, 108(2): 291-310. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291
|
[10] |
SNIBBE A C, MARKUS H R. You can't always get what you want: education attainment, agency and choice[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2005, 88(4): 703-720. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.703
|
[11] |
GUTHRIE G M, A social-psychological analysis of modernization in the Philippines[J]. Journal of cross-cultural psychology, 1977, 8(2): 177-206. doi: 10.1177/002202217782003
|
[12] |
HAMAMURA T, XU Q, DU Y. Culture, social class, and independence-interdependence: the case of Chinese adolescents[J]. International journal of psychology, 2013, 48(3): 344-351. doi: 10.1080/00207594.2011.647030
|
[13] |
KAMEDA T, TAKEZAWA M, HASTIE R. Where do social norms come from? The example of communal sharing[J]. Current directions in psychological science, 2005, 14(6): 331-334. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00392.x
|
[14] |
KITAYAMA S, PARK H, SEVINCER A T, et al. A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2009, 97(2): 236-255. doi: 10.1037/a0015999
|
[15] |
骆诚, 任孝鹏. 正在进行中的自愿拓疆运动与个体主义: 来自多种文化任务分析的证据[J]. 心理科学进展, 2018(11): 1928-1934. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XLXD201811003.htm
|
[16] |
TRIANDIS H C, GELFAND M J. Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1998, 74(1): 118-128. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118
|
[17] |
KIM K, CHO B. Development of an individualism collectivism scale revisited: a Korean sample[J]. Psychological reports, 2011, 108(2): 393-401. doi: 10.2466/02.07.17.21.PR0.108.2.393-401
|
[18] |
HAN J, LING J, LIM A. Emotional appeal in recruiting advertisement: unpacking national cultural differences[J]. Academy of management annual meeting proceedings, 2013(1): 10743.
|
[19] |
VARNUM M E W, KITAYAMA S. What's in a name? Popular names are less common on frontiers[J]. Psychological science, 2011, 22(2): 176-183. doi: 10.1177/0956797610395396
|
[20] |
徐江, 任孝鹏, 苏红. 个体主义/集体主义的影响因素: 生态视角[J]. 心理科学进展, 2016(8): 1309-1318. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XLXD201608017.htm
|
[21] |
BOMHOFF E, MAN-LI GU M. East Asia remains different: a comment on the index of "self-expression" values by inglehart and welzel[J]. Journal of cross-cultural psychology, 2012, 43(3): 373-383. doi: 10.1177/0022022111435096
|
[22] |
SHIVELY D H. Tradition and modernization in Japanese culture[M]. Princeton: Princeton University Press, 2015: 126-137.
|
[23] |
CASHDAN E, STEELE M. Pathogen prevalence, group bias, and collectivism in the standard cross-cultural sample[J]. Human nature, 2013, 24(1): 59-75. doi: 10.1007/s12110-012-9159-3
|
[24] |
SCHALLER M, DUNCAN L A. The behavioral immune system: its evolution and social psychological implications[M]//FORGAS J P, HASELTON M G, VON HIPPEL W. Evolution and the social mind: evolutionary psychology and social cognition. London: Psychology Press, 2007: 293-307.
|
[25] |
USKUL A K, KITAYAMA S, NISBETT R E. Ecocultural basis of cognition: farmers and fishermen are more holistic than herders[J]. Proceedings of the national academy of sciences, 2008, 105(25): 8552-8556. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0803874105
|
[26] |
TALLHELM T, ZHANG X, OISHI S, et al. Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture[J]. Science, 2014, 344: 603-608. doi: 10.1126/science.1246850
|
[27] |
KENRICK D T, GRISKEVICIUS V, NEUBERG S L, et al. Renovating the pyramid of needs: contemporary extensions built upon ancient foundations[J]. Perspectives on psychological science, 2010, 5(3): 292-314. doi: 10.1177/1745691610369469
|
[28] |
王婷, 植凤英, 陆禹同, 等. 侗歌经验对侗族中学生执行功能的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2019(9): 1040-1056. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XLXB201909007.htm
|
[29] |
王婷, 关宇霞, 关红英, 等. 鄂伦春族的视空间工作记忆能力优势: 生态环境和生产方式的影响[J]. 心理学报, 2018(10): 1094-1104. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-XLXB201810003.htm
|
[30] |
杨秀木, 齐玉龙, 申正付, 等. 家庭社会经济地位、职业价值观对医学生主观幸福感的影响[J]. 中国临床心理学杂志, 2015(1): 154-158. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-ZLCY201501036.htm
|
[31] |
JI L J, SCHWARZ N, NISBETT R E. Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2014, 87(1): 57-65.
|
[32] |
WANG C S, LEUNG K Y, SEE Y H M, et al. The effects of culture and friendship on rewarding honesty and punishing deception[J]. Journal of experimental social psychology, 2011, 47(6): 1295-1299. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2011.04.011
|
[33] |
JI L J, PENG K, NISBETT R E. Culture, control and perception of relationships in the environment[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 2000, 78(5): 943-955. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.943
|
[34] |
NEISSER U. The multiplicity of thought[J]. British journal of psychology, 1963, 54(1): 1-14.
|
[35] |
NORENZAYAN A. Evolution and transmitted culture[J]. Psychological inquiry, 2006, 17(2): 123-128.
|
[36] |
BERRY J W. Independance and conformity in subsistence-level societies[J]. Journal of personality and social psychology, 1967, 7(4): 415-418. doi: 10.1037/h0025231
|
[37] |
杜成材. 侗族大歌: 生计方式与文化模式的和谐之音[J]. 乐山师范学院学报, 2009(2): 118-121. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-LSSZ200902034.htm
|
[38] |
韩有峰. 鄂伦春族狩猎生产资料和组织形式[J]. 黑龙江民族丛刊, 1988(2): 86-90. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-HLMZ198802020.htm
|
[39] |
石英. 鄂伦春族传统居住空间环境探析[J]. 歌海, 2009(1): 118-119. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-GHAI200901057.htm
|
[40] |
朱莹, 屈芳竹, 仵娅婷. 鄂伦春族传统"住"与"居"的空间研究[J]. 建筑与文化, 2019(7): 93-95. https://www.cnki.com.cn/Article/CJFDTOTAL-JZYW201907034.htm
|
[41] |
BUCHTEL E E, NORENZAYAN A. Which should you use, intuition or logic? Cultural differences in injunctive norms about reasoning[J]. Asian journal of social psychology, 2008, 11(4): 264-273. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2008.00266.x
|
[42] |
梁漱溟. 中国文化要义[M]. 上海: 上海人民出版社, 2011: 122-125.
|
[43] |
费孝通. 乡土中国[M]. 上海: 上海世纪出版集团, 2005: 27.
|